Skip to Content

Department of Cognitive Science

Williams syndrome: Dissociation and mental structure

Mitch Parsell (mitch.parsell@mq.edu.au)
Human Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney

Abstract

Williams syndrome (WS) is a genetic disorder that, because of its unique cognitive profile, has been marshalled as evidence for the modularity of both language and social skills. But emerging evidence suggests the claims of modularity based on WS have been premature. This paper offers an examination of the recent literature on WS. It argues the literature gives little (if any) support for mental modularity. Rather than being rigidly modular, the WS brain is an extremely flexible organ that that co-opts available neural resource in a highly dynamic manner to cope in the world.

Citation details for this article:

Parsell, M. (2010). Williams Syndrome: Dissociation and Mental Structure. In W. Christensen, E. Schier, and J. Sutton (Eds.), ASCS09: Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the Australasian Society for Cognitive Science (pp. 277-284). Sydney: Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science.

DOI: 10.5096/ASCS200942
Download the PDF here

References

  1. Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Does the study of autism justify minimalist innate modularity? Learning and Individual Differences, 10, 179-191.
  2. Bellugi, U. Lai, Z. & Wang, P. P. (1997). Language, communication, and neural systems in Williams Syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 334-342.
  3. Bellugi, U., Adolph, R., Cassady, C., & Chiles, M. (1999). Towards the neural basis for hypersociability in a genetic syndrome. NeuroReport, 10, 1653-1657.
  4. Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics - Supplement, 6, 115-125.
  5. Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & St. George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1-6.
  6. Bonner, L. & Burton, M. (2004). 7-11-years-old children show an advantage for matching and recognizing the internal features of familiar faces: Evidence against development shift. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1019-1029.
  7. Brock, J. (2007). Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 97-127.
  8. Deruelle, C., Mancini, J., Livet, M., Cassé-Perrot, C., & de Schonen, S. (1999). Configural and local processing of faces in children with Williams syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 41, 276-298.
  9. Don, A., Schellenberg, E. G., & Rourke, B. P. (1999). Music and language skills of children with Williams syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 154-170.
  10. Donnai, D. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2000). Williams Syndrome: From Genotype Through to Cognitive Phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 97, 164-171.
  11. Doyle, T. F., Bellugi, U., Korenberg, J. R., & Graham, J. (2004). "Everybody in the world is my friend": Hypersociability in young children with Williams syndrome. American Journal on Medical Genetics, 124A, 263-273.
  12. Dykens, E. M., Rosner, B. A. & Ly, T. M. (2001). Drawing by individuals with Williams syndrome: Are people different from shapes? American Journal of Mental Retardation, 106, 94-107.
  13. Farran, E. K., Jarrold, C., & Gathercole, S. E. (2003). Divided attention, selective attention and drawing: Processing preferences in Williams syndrome are dependent on the task administered. Neuropsychologia, 41, 676-687.
  14. Francke, U. (1999). Williams-Beuren syndrome: Genes and mechanisms. Human Molecular Genetics, 8, 1947-1954.
  15. Frigerio, E., Burt, D. M., Gagliardi, C., Cioffi, G., Martelli, S., Perrett, D. I., & Borgatti, R. (2006). Is everybody always my friend? Perception of approachability in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 44, 254-259.
  16. Galaburda, A., & Bellugi, U. (2000). Multi-level analysis of cortical neuroanatomy in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl. 1), 74-88.
  17. Gerran, P. (2003). Nativism and Neuroconstructivism in the Explanation of Williams Syndrome. Biology and Philosophy, 18, 41-52
  18. Gosch, A., & Pankau, R. (1997). Personality characteristics and behaviour problems in individuals of different ages with Williams syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 39, 527-533.
  19. Grice, S. J., de Haan, M., Halit, H., Johnson, M. H., Csibra, G., Grant, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). ERP abnormalities of visual perception in Williams syndrome. NeuroReport, 14, 1773-1777.
  20. Grice, S. J., Spratling, M. W., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Halit, H., Csibra, G., de Haan, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2001). Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. Neuroreport, 12, 2697-2700.
  21. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental approach to cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  22. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389-398.
  23. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). The tortuous route from genes to behaviour: A neuroconstructivist approach. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioural Neuroscience, 6, 9-17.
  24. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Nativism Versus Neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the Study of Developmental Disorders. Developmental Psychology, 45, 56-63.
  25. Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196-208.
  26. Karmiloff-Smith, A., Scerif, G. & Ansari, D. (2003). Double Dissociations in Developmental Disorders? Theoretically Misconceived, Empirically Dubious. Cortex 39, 161-163.
  27. Klein, A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., & Brown, F. R. (1990). Hyperacusis and otitis media in people with WS. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 339-344.
  28. Laws, G., & Bishop, D. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: A comparison with Down's syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 45-64.
  29. Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Lincoln, A. & Bellugi, U. (2005). Aversion, awareness, and attraction: investigating claims of hyperacusis in the Williams syndrome phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 514-523.
  30. Levitin, D.J., & Bellugi, U. (1998). Musical abilities in people with WS. Music Perception, 15, 357-389.
  31. Levitin, D. J., & Bellugi, U. (1999). Music cognition and Williams Syndrome. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 22-35.
  32. Losh, M., Bellugi, U., Reilly, J., & Anderson, D. (2000). Narrative as a social engagement tool: The excessive use of evaluations in narratives from children with Williams syndrome. Narrative Inquiry, 10, 265-290.
  33. Martens, M. A., Wilson, S. J. & Reutens, D. S. (2008). Research Review: Williams Syndrome: A critical review of the cognitive, behavioural, and neuroanatomical phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 576-608.
  34. Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B. P., Bertrand, J., Kwinty, S., Appelbaum, L. G., & Rice, C. E. (2003). Attentional characteristics of infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome during triadic interactions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 243-268.
  35. Mills, D. L., Alvarez, T. D., St George, M., Appelbaum, L. G., Bellugi, U., & Neville, H. (2000). Electrophysiological studies of face processing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 47-64.
  36. Parsell, M. (2009). Quinean Social Skills: Empirical Evidence From Eye-Gaze Against Information Encapsulation. Biology and Philosophy, 24, 1-19
  37. Peoples R, Franke Y, Wang Y. K, Pérez-Jurado L, Paperna T, Cisco M, Francke U. (2000). A physical map, including a BAC/PAC clone contig, of the Williams-Beuren syndrome deletion region at 7q11.23. American Journal of Human Genetics, 66, 47-68.
  38. Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: The ingredients of language. London: Weidenfield & Nicolson.
  39. Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. London: Penguin.
  40. Porter, M. A. & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive Heterogeneity in Williams Syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 275-306.
  41. Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Global and local processing in Williams syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome: Perception, attention and construction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30, 771-790.
  42. Porter, M. A., Coltheart, M. & Langdon, R. (2007). The neuropsychological basis of hypersociability in Williams and Down syndrome. Neurosychologia, 45, 2839-2849.
  43. Porter, M. A., Coltheart, M. & Langdon, R. (2008). Theory of Mind in Williams Syndrome Assessed Using a Nonverbal Task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 806-814
  44. Reilly, J., Klima, E. & Bellugi, U. (1990). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Developmental Psychopathology , 2, 367-391.
  45. Riby, D.M., Doherty-Sneddon, G. & Bruce, V. (2008). Atypical unfamiliar face processing in Williams syndrome: what can it tell us about typical familiarity effects? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 13, 47-58.
  46. Samson, D. & Pillon, A. (2003). A case of impaired knowledge for fruit and vegetables. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 373-400
  47. Stojanovik, V. & van Ewijk, L. (2008). Do children with Williams syndrome have unusual vocabularies? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21, 18-34.
  48. Stojanovik, V. (2006). Social interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19, 157-173
  49. Stojanovik, V., Perkins, M. & Howard, S. (2001) Language and conversational abilities in Williams Syndrome. How good is good? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 234-239.
  50. Stojanovik, V., Perkins, M. & Howard, S. (2004). Williams syndrome and Specific Language Impairment do not support claims for developmental double dissociations and innate modularity. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 403-424.
  51. Strømme P, Bjørnstad, P. G. & Ramstad K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 269-271.
  52. Tager-Flusberg, H., Boshart, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Reading the windows to the soul: Evidence of domain-specific sparing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 631-639.
  53. Thal, D., Bates, E., & Bellugi, U. (1989). Language and cognition in two children with Williams syndrome. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 32, 489-500.
  54. Udwin, O. (1990). A survey of adults with Williams syndrome and idiopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32, 129-141.
  55. Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1991). A cognitive and behavioral phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 232-244.
  56. Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., Tonucci, & Vicari, S. (2003). Early linguistic abilities of Italian children with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 33-58.
  57. Wang, Y. K., Samos, C. H., Peoples R, Pérez-Jurado, L. A., Nusse R, & Francke U. (1997). A novel human homologue of the Drosophila frizzled wnt receptor gene binds wingless protein and is in the Williams syndrome deletion at 7q11.23. Human Molecular Genetics, 6, 465-472.
  58. Young, A. W., Hay, D. C., McWeeney, K. H. Flude, B. M. & Ellis, A. W. (1985). Matching familiar and unfamiliar faces on internal and external features. Perception, 14, 737-746.

Further Information

Seminars

Who is Visiting

Contact Details

Telephone: (02) 9850 9599
Fax : (02) 9850 6059
Email : cogsci@mq.edu.au
Web : www.cogsci.mq.edu.au